Bark (sound)

A dog barking

A bark is a sound most often produced by dogs. Other animals that make this noise include, but are not limited to, wolves,[1] coyotes, foxes, seals, frogs, and barking owls. "Bark" is also a verb that describes the sound of many canids. [2]

Definition

[edit]

There is no precise, consistent, and functional acoustic definition for barking, but researchers classify barks according to several criteria.[3] Researchers at the University of Massachusetts Amherst and Hampshire College have defined a bark as a short, abrupt vocalization that is relatively loud and high-pitched, changes in frequency, and often repeats rapidly in succession.[2]

Barking in dogs

[edit]

Dog barking is distinct from wolf barking. Wolf barks represent only 2.4% of all wolf vocalizations, in warning, defense, and protest.[4][5] In contrast, dogs bark in many social situations, with acoustic communication in dogs being described as hypertrophic.[6] While wolf barks tend to be brief and isolated, dog barking is often repetitive.[7]

One hypothesis for why dogs bark more than wolves is that dogs developed vocal communication as a result of their domestication.[7] Domestication can alter a species and affect its physical and physiological characteristics.[8] Domesticated breeds show vast physical differences from their wild counterparts, an evolution that suggests neoteny, or the retention of juvenile characteristics in adults.[9] The behaviour of adult dogs also shows puppy-like characteristics: dogs act submissive, whine, and bark often.

The frequency of barking in dogs compared to wolves could also be the product of the different social environments of dogs. From a young age, humans may be among a dog's primary social contacts, an environment that presents different stimuli than would be found by wolves in the wild. Intruders may frequent the boundaries of a captive dog's territory, thus triggering the bark response as a warning. Dogs also live in densely populated urban areas, allowing for more opportunities for socialization. For example, kennelled dogs may have increased barking due to a desire to facilitate social behaviour. Dogs' close relationship with humans also renders dogs reliant on humans, even for basic needs. Barking can be used as a way to attract attention, and any positive response exhibited by the owners reinforces the behaviour. For example, if a dog barks to get food and the owner feeds it, they are conditioned to continue said behaviour.[10]

Types

[edit]

The purposes of barking by domestic dogs is a controversial topic. While barking is suggested to be "non-communicative",[11] data suggests that it may indeed be a means of expression that became increasingly sophisticated during domestication. Due to the lack of consensus over whether or not dogs communicate using their barks, not much research has been done to categorize the different types of barking in dogs. Some existing research has been criticized by Feddersen-Petersen as "lack[ing] objectivity". Using sonographic methods, Feddersen-Petersen identified several distinct types of barks, then analysed them for meanings, functions, and emotions. He separated dog barks into subgroups based on said sonographic data:

Bark Characteristics Behavior
Infantile bark (pup yelp) Harmonic Emitted spontaneously in protest or as a distress call
Harmonic play bark Mixed sounds involving "concurrent superimposition" of growls, noisy bark After barking, play behavior was often observed.
"Christmas tree" bark Sonogram displayed "Christmas tree" effect. There is a "sequential loss of overtones". Seen in German Shepherds and Alaskan Malamutes.
Noisy overlappings Short, overlapping sounds Seen in poodles.
Pure harmonic sound Often accompanied with play behavior.
Specific vibrato-growl
Noisy bark Agonistic contexts only. Seen in Alaskan Malamutes.
Play-solicited barks Often combined with growls, other bark subunits. Matched with play behavior
Noisy play bark Harsh, short sound. Low-pitched, with an extremely short, sharp rise. Associated with more harsh play-fighting seen often in American Staffordshire Terriers and Bull Terriers. "Often show[ed] changeovers to aggressive interactions."
Threat bark Short, low-pitched sound.
Warning bark Short, low-pitched sound.

Not all breeds demonstrate every subgroup of barking. Instead, there is significant variance in vocalization between different breeds. Poodles show the least of all barking subunits. Barking in wolves was observed as less diverse. For example, wolf barks are rarely harmonic, tending instead to be noisy.[12]

There is some evidence that humans can determine the suspected emotions of dogs while listening to barks emitted during specific situations. Humans scored the emotions of dogs performing these barks very similarly and in ways that made sense according to the context. In one example, when subjects were played a recording of a dog tied alone to a tree, a situation in which one could infer that the dog would be distressed, the human listeners tended to rank the bark as having a high level of despair. Some suggest that this may be evidence that dog barks have evolved to be a form of communication with humans, since humans can determine a dog's needs by listening to their vocalizations.[13] Further studies have found that the acoustic structure of a bark "[varies] considerably with context."[14] These studies suggest that barks are more than just random sounds, and hold some communicative purpose.

Excessive barking

[edit]

Causes and solutions

[edit]
Signaling to a dog with the palm of the hand is prescribed as a way to address a dog that is alert barking
Splitting is prescribed as a way to address a dog that is alert barking

Barking is a normal behaviour for dogs. What counts as excessive barking is a subjective idea.[15] Excessive dog barking can be a nuisance and a common problem that dog owners or their neighbors may face.[16] Excessive barking indicates an underlying problem, a trigger or the dog lacking exercise or mental stimulation. Reasons for excessive barking can be, among others, pain, fear, boredom, frustration, separation anxiety or territorial behaviour (warning about potential threats).[15]

Different kinds of barking require different approaches to reduction.[17]

Common approaches are:

  1. Attempting to understand, and if possible eliminate, the causes of barking.
  2. Using positive training methods to correct the behavior. Dogs may bark from anxiety or stress, so punishment can often cause problems by reinforcing a cycle of bad behavior. Positive approaches can include:
    • Repeated exposure to stimuli whilst calming the dog and persuading it to remain quiet.
    • Distraction as the stimulus happens, through treats, praise, or similar.
    • Reshaping via clicker training (a form of operant conditioning) or other means to obtain barking behavior on command, and then shaping the control to gain command over silence.
  3. In her 2008 book Barking: The Sound of a Language,[18] Turid Rugaas explains that barking is a way a dog communicates. She suggests signaling back to show the dog that the dog's attempts to communicate have been acknowledge and to calm a dog down. She suggests the use of a hand signal and a Calming Signal called Splitting.
  4. Seeking professional advice from local organizations, dog trainers, or veterinarians.

If all other methods have been tried, a last effort is using a bark collar. These collars might release an unpleasant smell or mild to painful shock. Various bark collars have been praised and criticized; some are considered inhumane by various people and groups. Critics consider them torturous and compare their use to electrocution. However, most Societies for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals agree[citation needed] that in a last resort an electric collar is better than euthanasia if it comes to an ultimatum, for a stubborn dog that will not stop any other way. Most agree that understanding the communication and retraining by reward is the most effective and humane method.

Surgical debarking

[edit]

The controversial surgical procedure known as 'debarking' or 'bark softening' is a veterinary procedure for modifying the voice box so that a barking dog will make a significantly reduced noise. It is considered a last resort by some owners claiming that it is better than euthanasia, seizure, or legal problems if the matter has proven incapable of being corrected any other way.

Debarking is illegal in many European states and is opposed by animal welfare organizations.

Breeds

[edit]

The Huntaway is a working dog that has been selectively bred to drive stock (usually sheep) by using its voice. It was bred in New Zealand, and is bred based on ability rather than appearance or lineage.

Naturally "barkless" dog breeds

[edit]

Compared to most domestic dogs, the bark of a dingo is short and monosyllabic.[19] During observations, the Australian dingo's bark has a small variability; sub-groups of bark types, common among domestic dogs, could not be found. Furthermore, just 5% of the observed vocalizations consisted of barking. Australian dingoes bark only in swooshing noises or a mixture of atonal and tonal. Also, barking is almost exclusively used for giving warnings. Warn-barking in a homotypical sequence and a kind of "warn-howling" in a heterotypical one have also been observed. The bark-howling starts with several barks and then fades into a rising and ebbing howl and is probably, similarly to coughing, used to warn the puppies and members of the pack. Dingoes also emit a sort of "wailing" sound, which they use most often when approaching a water hole, probably to warn already present dingoes.[20] According to current knowledge, it is impossible to get Australian dingoes to bark more often by making them associate with other domestic dogs. However, Alfred Brehm reported a dingo that learned the more "typical" form of barking and knew how to use it, while its brother did not.[21] Whether dingoes bark or bark-howl less overall is unknown.[22]

The extinct Hare Indian dog of northern Canada was not known to bark in its native homeland, though puppies born in Europe learned how to imitate the barking of other dogs.[23] When hurt or afraid, it howled like a wolf, and when curious, it made a sound described as a growl building up to a howl.[24]

The Basenji of central Africa produces a yodel-like sound due to its unusually shaped larynx.[25] This trait also gives the Basenji the nickname "Barkless Dog".[26]

Barking in other animals

[edit]

Besides dogs and wolves, other canines like coyotes and jackals can bark.[2] Their barks are similar to those of wolves and dogs.

The warning bark of a fox is higher and more drawn out than barks of other canids.

There are non-canine species with vocalizations that may be described as barking. Because the alarm call of the muntjac resembles a dog's bark, they are sometimes known as "barking deer". Eared seals are also known to bark. Prairie dogs employ complex communication involving barks and rhythmic chirps.[27] Various bird species produce vocalizations that include the canonical features of barking, especially when avoiding predators.[2] Some primate species, notably gorillas, can and do vocalize in short barks.

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ L. David Mech; Luigi Boitani (1 October 2010). Wolves: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation. University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-51698-1.
  2. ^ a b c d Lord, Kathryn., Feinstein, Mark., Coppinger, Raymond. Barking and mobbing. Archived 29 June 2011 at the Wayback Machine Behavioural Processes. 2009.
  3. ^ Not Only Dogs, But Deer, Monkeys And Birds Bark To Deal With Conflict. Science Daily. 15 July 2009.
  4. ^ Schassburger, R.M. (1987). "Wolf vocalization: An integrated model of structure, motivation, and ontogeny". In H. Frank (ed.). Man and Wolf. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Dr. W. Junk.
  5. ^ Coscia, E.M. (1995). Ontogeny of timber wolf vocalizations: Acoustic properties and behavioral contexts (PhD Dissertation). Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada: Dalhousie University. hdl:10222/55083.
  6. ^ Fedderson-Peterson, D.U. (2000). "Vocalization of European wolves (Canus lupus lupus L.) and various dog breeds (Canus lupus f., fam.)". Arch. Tierz. 4. Kiel, Germany: Institut für Haustierkunde, Christian-Albrechts-University: 387–397.
  7. ^ a b Coppinger, R.; M. Feinstein (1991). "'Hark! Hark! The dogs do bark...' and bark and hark". Smithsonian. 21: 119–128.
  8. ^ Belyaev, D.K.; I.Z. Plyusnina; L.N. Trut (1984). "Domestication in the silver fox (Vulpus fulvus desm.) – changes in physiological boundaries of the sensitive period of primary socialization". Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 13 (4): 359–370. doi:10.1016/0168-1591(85)90015-2.
  9. ^ Fox, M.W. (1986). Saunders, W.B. (ed.). "The influence of domestication upon behavior of animals". Abnormal Behaviour in Animals. Philadelphia: 179–187.
  10. ^ Fox, M.W. (1971). "Behavior of wolves and related canids". Malabar, FL. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  11. ^ Coppinger, R; M. Feinstein. "'Hark! Hark! The dogs do bark...' and bark and bark". Smithsonian (21): 119–128.
  12. ^ Feddersen-Petersen, D.U. (2000). "Vocalization of European wolves (Canus lupus lupus L.) and various dog breeds (Canus lupus f., fam.)". Arch. Tierz. 4. Kiel, Germany: Institut für Haustierkunde, Christian-Albrechts-University: 387–397.
  13. ^ Pongrácz, P; Molnár, C.; Miklósi, Á.; Csányi, V. (2005). "Human Listeners Are Able to Classify Dog (Canis familiaris) Barks Recorded in Different Situations". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2. 119 (2): 136–144. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.136. PMID 15982157.
  14. ^ Yin, S (2002). "A New Perspective on Barking in Dogs (Canis familiaris)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2. 116 (2): 189–193. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.116.2.189. PMID 12083615.
  15. ^ a b "What causes dogs to bark excessively?". RSPCA Knowledgebase. Retrieved 7 January 2024.
  16. ^ Richard Murray and Helen Penridge. Dogs in the Urban Environment. Chiron Media 1992, ISBN 0-646-07157-2, pp. 21–22.
  17. ^ Mordecai Siegal and Matt Margolis. When Good Dogs Do Bad Things: Proven solutions to 30 common problems. Little, Brown and Co. 1986, ISBN 0-316-79012-5, pp. 33–44.
  18. ^ Rugaas, Turid (2008). Barking : the sound of a language. Wenatchee, Wash.: Dogwise Pub. ISBN 978-1929242511.
  19. ^ Free Ranging Dogs - Stray, Feral or Wild?. Guillaume de Lavigne. 19 March 2015. ISBN 9781326219529. Retrieved 11 March 2020.
  20. ^ Laurie Corbett (2004). "Dingo" (PDF). Canids: Foxes, Wolves, Jackals and Dogs. International Union for Conservation of Nature and Natural Resources. Retrieved 8 April 2009.
  21. ^ Brehms Tierleben (in German). Leipzig, Wien: Bibliographisches Institut. 1900. pp. 82–85.
  22. ^ Feddersen-Petersen, Dorit Urd (2008). Ausdrucksverhalten beim Hund (in German). Stuttgart: Franckh-Kosmos Verlags-GmbH & Co. KG. ISBN 978-3-440-09863-9.
  23. ^ The Gardens and Menagerie of the Zoological Society, Published, with the Sanction of the Council, Under the Superintendence of the Secretary and Vice-secretary of the Society, by Edward Turner Bennett, Zoological Society of London, William Harvey, Illustrated by John Jackson, William Harvey, G. B., S. S., Thomas Williams, Robert Edward Branston, George Thomas Wright. Published by Printed by C. Whittingham, 1830.
  24. ^ Fauna Boreali-americana, Or, The Zoology of the Northern Parts of British America: Containing Descriptions of the Objects of Natural History Collected on the Late Northern Land Expeditions, Under Command of Captain Sir John Franklin, R.N. By John Richardson, William Swainson, William Kirby, published by J. Murray, 1829.
  25. ^ Adapted from the book "Why Pandas Do Handstands," 2006, by Augustus Brown.
  26. ^ Tudor-Williams, Veronica (May 1988). "1945 Letter from Africa". The Basenji. BCOA African Stock Project. Retrieved 21 May 2019.
  27. ^ Walker, Matt. Burrowing US prairie dogs use complex language. BBC Earth News. 2 February 2010.
[edit]