Pupillometry

From Wikipedia the free encyclopedia

Pupillometry, the measurement of pupil size and reactivity, is a key part of the clinical neurological exam for patients with a wide variety of neurological injuries. It is also used in psychology.[1][2]

Pupillometry in critical care

[edit]

For more than 100 years, clinicians have evaluated the pupils of patients with suspected or known brain injury or impaired consciousness to monitor neurological status and trends, checking for pupil size and reactivity to light.[3] In fact, before the advent of electricity, doctors checked a patient’s reaction to light using a candle.

Today, clinicians routinely evaluate pupils as a component of the neurological examination and monitoring of critically ill patients, including patients with traumatic brain injury and stroke.[4][5][6] In 2016, Couret et Al. showed that "Standard practice in pupillary monitoring yields inaccurate data that Automated quantitative pupillometry is a more reliable method with which to collect pupillary measurements at the bedside.[7] In 2019, the first smartphone based pupillometer was released as an accurate and economical way to determine pupil size and dynamic response objectively.[8] However, another study has shown the necessary use of an opaque eyecup as pupillary light reflex is affected by ambient light. [7] It is important to mention that certain pupillometers and all smartphones do not have this particular feature.

Pupillometer
NeuroLight pupillometer and its QPi score

Patient care and outcome

[edit]

Numerous studies have shown the importance of pupil evaluation in the clinical setting, and pupillary information is used extensively in patient management and as an indication for possible medical intervention.

Patients who undergo prompt intervention after a new finding of pupil abnormality have a better chance of recovery.[9]

Alterations of the pupil light reflex, size of the pupil, and anisocoria (unequal pupils) are correlated with outcomes of patients with traumatic brain injury.[10][2][11][12][13][14][15][16][17][18][19][20] Blood flow imaging has shown that pupil changes are highly correlated with brainstem oxygenation and perfusion,[19][18][21] and anisocoria can be an indicator of a pathological process or neurological dysfunction.[18][22][23]

Investigators have used pupil size and reactivity as fundamental parameters of outcome predictive models in conjunction with other clinical information such as age, mechanism of injury, and Glasgow Coma Scale,[21][24][25] and have correlated the models with the presence and location of intracranial mass lesions.[11]

The National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale (NIHSS) uses pupillary response as a systematic assessment tool to provide a quantitative measure of stroke-related neurologic deficit and to evaluate acuity of stroke patients, determine appropriate treatment, and predict patient outcome.[26]

Manual vs. automated pupil assessment

[edit]

Traditionally, pupil measurements have been performed in a subjective manner by using a penlight or flashlight to manually evaluate pupil reactivity (sPLR, "s" stands for standard) and using a pupil gauge to estimate pupil size. However, manual pupillary assessment is subject to significant inaccuracies and inconsistencies. Studies have shown inter-examiner disagreement in the manual evaluation of pupillary reaction to be as high as 39 percent.[1][2][4][5][27][28][29][30] Automated pupillometry involves the use of a pupillometer, a portable, handheld device that provides a reliable and objective measurement of pupillary size, symmetry, and reactivity through measurement of the pupil light reflex (qPLR). sPLR is opposed to quantitative PLR (qPLR) that is provided by an automated pupillometer. qPLR[31] corresponds to the percentage of pupillary constriction to a calibrated light stimulus. Pupillometers before 2018 predominately used infrared cameras to observe pupil diameter. Then, in 2019, advancements in machine learning have enabled visual spectrum pupillometry using a smartphone. When measuring the pupillary light reflex, it's important to use an opaque eyecup to get accurate results.[7] This is because the measurement can be affected by ambient light. It's worth noting that some devices, such as smartphones and certain pupillometers, lack this ability. Therefore, using an eyecup is even more necessary. Overall, using an eyecup helps ensure precise measurements of the pupillary light reflex. Numeric scales allow for a more rigorous interpretation and classification of the pupil response and are a primary feature of both hardware and software based pupillometers.

NPi-300 automated infrared pupillometer (NeurOptics, Inc.)

Automated pupillometry removes subjectivity from the pupillary evaluation, providing more accurate and trendable pupil data, and allowing earlier detection of changes for more timely patient treatment. By using automated pupillometers and algorithms such as QPi score (Quantitative Pupillometry Index) or Reflex's "Reflex Score", doctors can easily and objectively assess pupil reactivity that could otherwise be missed by manual assessment. Automated pupillometers have been proven to be more effective than manual pupil assessment.

With an automated pupillometer and an algorithm analyzing the pupil continuously for 5 seconds, the Quantitative Pupillometry Index (QPi) can measure pupillary reactivity and provides a numerical value. It provides objective data and can detect subtle changes that might not be apparent to the naked eye.  Its quantitative nature provides objective and more reliable assessment.   Moreover, it is color-coded for a quick clinical interpretation. It displays through a qualitative scale a quantitative interval for each color associated with its number.[32]

Mobile visual spectrum automated pupillometers have been proven effective as an alternative to infrared pupillometers that typically command a higher cost.[33][10] Controversy has risen around infrared pupillometers as some of them are routinely incapable of measuring hippus, a natural pupillary phenomenon, which has professionals concluding that NeurOptics' devices fit a curve to measured data. The NeuroLight pupillometer (IDMED), on the other hand, provides this pupillary unrest in ambient light (PUAL) function, which is described as a consistent indicator of opioid effect and is the gold standard in this field of research.[34][35] Infrared pupillometers use an eye guard that is placed on a subject's orbit or zygomatic bone and uses a fixed distance calibration to determine pupil size which has further brought into question the validity of fixed distance measures as the human population varies widely in skull structure. The NeuroLight and NPi pupillometers are both devices for measuring pupils but differ significantly in terms of ergonomics and functionality. The main distinction lies in the NPi’s use of a transparent eye guard that contains an electronic component for patient identification and results recording, making it unique to each patient. This consumable that allows ambient light to pass through may result in data reproducibility issues and increased costs. NeuroLight, in contrast, comes with a touchscreen display and employs a reusable opaque eyecup that isolates the eye from ambient light. This design feature not only enhances the accuracy of the pupillary measurements[7] but also reduces the overall cost of usage to the initial purchase of the device.

According to the new American Heart Association guidelines, most deaths attributable to post-cardiac arrest brain injury are due to active withdrawal of life-sustaining treatment based on a predicted poor neurological outcome. The NPi and automated pupillometry have recently been included in the updated 2020 American Heart Association (AHA) Guidelines for Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation (CPR) and Emergency Cardiovascular Care (ECC) as an object measurement supporting brain injury prognosis in patients following cardiac arrest.[36]

A study published in the Journal of Neurosurgery found that automated pupillometers may signal an early warning of potential delayed cerebral ischemia and enable preemptive escalation of care.[37]

The American Journal of Critical Care revealed that critical care and neurosurgical nurses consistently underestimated pupil size, were unable to identify anisocoria, and incorrectly assessed pupil reactivity (sPLR). It concluded that automated pupillometry is a necessary tool for accuracy and consistency, and that it might facilitate earlier detection of subtle pupil changes, allowing more effective and timely diagnostic and treatment interventions.[1]

In addition, a study from The University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center compared 2,329 manual pupillary exams performed simultaneously by two examiners (neurology and neurosurgery attending and resident physicians, staff nurses, and mid-level practitioners) under identical conditions and showed low inter-examiner reliability.[28][29] The American Association of Critical-Care Nurses (AACN) Procedure Manual for High Acuity, Progressive and Critical Care, 7th Edition, and the American Association of Neuroscience Nurses (AANN) Core Curriculum for Neuroscience Nursing, 6th Edition, now include sections illustrating how use of a pupillometer removes subjectivity and allows pupillary reactivity to be trended in a consistent, objective, and quantifiable way. The AACN Procedure Manual, which was extensively reviewed by more than 100 experts in critical care nursing, is the authoritative reference for procedures performed in critical care settings, and the AANN curriculum is a comprehensive resource for practicing neuroscience nurses.

Advancements in mobile-based automated pupillometry have been made in recent years to accommodate for the growing number of mobile phones being used in healthcare. Notably, brightlamp, Inc. has secured the first intellectual property relating to mobile quantitative pupillometry.

Pupillometry in psychology

[edit]

Stimulants

[edit]

Photographs

[edit]

Hess and Polt (1960)[38] presented pictures of semi-naked adults and babies to adults (four men and two women). Pupils of both sexes dilated after seeing pictures of people of the opposite sex. In females, the difference in pupil size occurred also after seeing pictures of babies and mothers with babies. This examination showed that pupils react not only to the changes of intensity of light (pupillary light reflex) but also reflect arousal or emotions.

In 1965 Hess, Seltzer and Shlien[39] examined pupillary responses in heterosexual and homosexual males. Results showed a greater pupil dilation to pictures of the opposite sex for heterosexuals and to pictures of the same sex for homosexuals.

According to T.M. Simms (1967),[40] pupillary responses of males and females were greater when they were exposed to pictures of the opposite sex.[41] In another study, Nunnally and colleagues (1967)[42] found that seeing slides rated as 'very pleasant' was associated with greater pupil dilation as seeing slides rated as neutral or very unpleasant.

Infants showed greater pupil size when they saw pictures of faces than when they saw geometric shapes,[41][43][44] and greater dilation after seeing pictures of the infant's mother than pictures of a stranger.[43]

Cognitive load

[edit]

Pupillary responses can reflect activation of the brain allocated to cognitive tasks. Greater pupil dilation is associated with increased processing in the brain.[45] Vacchiano and colleagues (1968) found that pupillary responses were associated with visual exposure to words with high, neutral or low value. Presented low-value words were associated with dilation, and high-value words with constriction of a pupil.[46] In decision-making tasks dilation increased before the decision as a function of cognitive load.[47][48] In an experiment about short-term serial memory, students heard strings of words and were asked to repeat them. Greater pupil diameter was observed after the items were heard (depending on how many items were heard), and decreased after items were repeated.[49] The more difficult the task, the greater pupil diameter observed from the time preceding the solution [50] until the task was completed.[51] While these discoveries from the 1960s sparked renewed interest in the psychological significance of pupil size, research had substantially earlier identified the relationship between pupil size and effort.[52][53]

Long-term memory

[edit]

The pupil response reflects long-term memory processes both at encoding, predicting the success of memory formation[54] and at retrieval, reflecting different recognition outcomes.[55]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b c Kerr, R (2016). "Underestimation of pupil size by critical care and neurosurgical nurses". American Journal of Critical Care. 25 (3): 213–219. doi:10.4037/ajcc2016554. PMID 27134226. S2CID 8564670.
  2. ^ a b c Olson, D (2015). "The use of automated pupillometry in critical care". Critical Care Nursing Clinics of North America. 28 (1): 101–107. doi:10.1016/j.cnc.2015.09.003. PMID 26873763.
  3. ^ Loewenfeld, I. (1993). "The Pupil: Anatomy, Physiology, and Clinical Application". Ames: Iowa State University Press.
  4. ^ a b Meeker, M (2005). "Pupil examination: validity and clinical utility of an automated pupillometer". J Neurosci Nurs. 37 (1): 34–40. doi:10.1097/01376517-200502000-00006. PMID 15794443.
  5. ^ a b Wilson, S (1988). "Determining interrater reliability of nurses' assessments of pupillary size and reaction". J Neurosci Nurs. 20 (3): 189–192. doi:10.1097/01376517-198806000-00011. PMID 2968419. S2CID 24775913.
  6. ^ Chestnut, R (2000). Management and Prognosis of Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. Part II: Early Indicators of Prognosis in Severe Traumatic Brain Injury. Brain Trauma Foundation, American Association of Neurological Surgeons, Joint Section on Neurotrauma and Critical Care. pp. 153–255.
  7. ^ a b c d Couret, David; Simeone, Pierre; Freppel, Sebastien; Velly, Lionel (2019-04-01). "The effect of ambient-light conditions on quantitative pupillometry: a history of rubber cup". Neurocritical Care. 30 (2): 492–493. doi:10.1007/s12028-018-0664-z. ISSN 1556-0961.
  8. ^ Service, Purdue News. "Improved Reflex app from brightlamp unlocks 'the diagnostic power of the pupil,' provides diagnostic data for concussions in seconds". www.purdue.edu. Retrieved 2022-01-31.
  9. ^ Clusmann, H (2001). "Fixed and dilated pupils after trauma, stroke, and previous intracranial surgery: management and outcome". J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 71 (2): 175–181. doi:10.1136/jnnp.71.2.175. PMC 1737504. PMID 11459888.
  10. ^ a b Carrick, Frederick Robert; Azzolino, Sergio F.; Hunfalvay, Melissa; Pagnacco, Guido; Oggero, Elena; D’Arcy, Ryan C. N.; Abdulrahman, Mahera; Sugaya, Kiminobu (October 2021). "The Pupillary Light Reflex as a Biomarker of Concussion". Life. 11 (10): 1104. doi:10.3390/life11101104. PMC 8537991. PMID 34685475.
  11. ^ a b Braakman, R; Gelpke, G; Habbema, J; Maas, A; Minderhoud, J (1980). "Systemic selection of prognostic features in patients with severe head injury". Neurosurgery. 6 (4): 362–370. doi:10.1227/00006123-198004000-00002. PMID 7393417.
  12. ^ Chen, J; Gombart, Z; Rogers, S; Gardiner, S; Cecil, S; Bullock, R (2011). "Pupillary reactivity as an early indicator of increased intracranial pressure: the introduction of the neurological pupil index". Surg Neurol Int. 2: 82. doi:10.4103/2152-7806.82248. PMC 3130361. PMID 21748035.
  13. ^ Chesnut, R; Gautille, T; Blunt, B; Klauber, M; Marshall, L (1994). "The localizing value of asymmetry in pupillary size in severe head injury: relation to lesion type and location". Neurosurgery. 34 (5): 840–845. doi:10.1097/00006123-199405000-00008. PMID 8052380.
  14. ^ Choi, S; Narayan, R; Anderson, R; Ward, J (1988). "Enhanced specificity of prognosis in severe head injury". J Neurosurg. 69 (3): 381–385. doi:10.3171/jns.1988.69.3.0381. PMID 3404236.
  15. ^ Levin, H; Gary, H; Eisenberg, H; et al. (1990). "Neurobehavioral outcome 1 year after severe head injury. Experience of the Traumatic Coma Data Bank". J Neurosurg. 73 (5): 699–709. doi:10.3171/jns.1990.73.5.0699. PMID 2213159.
  16. ^ Marshall, L; Gautille, T; Klauber, M; et al. (1991). "The outcome of severe closed head injury". J Neurosurg. 75: 28–36. doi:10.3171/sup.1991.75.1s.0s28.
  17. ^ Ritter, A; Muizelaar, J; Barnes, T; et al. (1999). "Brain stem blood flow, pupillary response, and outcome in patients with severe head injuries". Neurosurgery. 44 (5): 941–948. doi:10.1097/00006123-199905000-00005. PMID 10232526.
  18. ^ a b c Sakas, D; Bullock, M; Teasdale, G (1995). "One-year outcome following craniotomy for traumatic hematoma in patients with fixed dilated pupils". J Neurosurg. 82 (6): 961–965. doi:10.3171/jns.1995.82.6.0961. PMID 7760198.
  19. ^ a b Taylor, W; Chen, J; Meltzer, H; et al. (2003). ""Quantitative pupillometry, a new technology " normative data and preliminary observations in patients with acute head injury". J Neurosurg. 98 (1): 205–213. doi:10.3171/jns.2003.98.1.0205. PMID 12546375.
  20. ^ Tien, H; Cunha, J; Wu, S; et al. (2006). "Do trauma patients with a Glasgow Coma Scale score of 3 and bilateral fixed and dilated pupils have any chance of survival?". J Trauma. 60 (2): 274–278. doi:10.1097/01.ta.0000197177.13379.f4. PMID 16508482.
  21. ^ a b Zhao, D; Weil, M; Tang, W; Klouche, K; Wann, S (2001). "Pupil diameter and light reaction during cardiac arrest and resuscitation". Crit. Care Med. 4 (29): 825–828. doi:10.1097/00003246-200104000-00029. PMID 11373477. S2CID 22441487.
  22. ^ Andrews, B; Pitts, L (1991). "Functional recovery after traumatic transtentorial herniation". Neurosurgery. 29 (2): 227–231. doi:10.1227/00006123-199108000-00010. PMID 1886660.
  23. ^ Petridis, A. K.; Dörner, L.; Doukas, A.; Eifrig, S.; Barth, H.; Mehdorn, M. (2009). "Acute Subdural Hematoma in the Elderly; Clinical and CT Factors Influencing the Surgical Treatment Decision". Central European Neurosurgery. 70 (2): 73–78. doi:10.1055/s-0029-1224096. PMID 19711259.
  24. ^ Marmarou, A; Lu, J; Butcher, I; et al. (2007). ""Prognostic value of the Glasgow Coma Scale and pupil reactivity in traumatic brain injury assessed pre-hospital and on enrollment " an IMPACT analysis". J Neurotrauma. 24 (2): 270–280. doi:10.1089/neu.2006.0029. PMID 17375991.
  25. ^ Narayan, R; Greenberg, R; Miller, J; et al. (1981). ""Improved confidence of outcome prediction in severe head injury " A comparative analysis of the clinical examination, multimodality evoked potentials, CT scanning, and intracranial pressure". J Neurosurg. 54 (6): 751–762. doi:10.3171/jns.1981.54.6.0751. PMID 7241184.
  26. ^ NIH Stroke Scale (NIHSS). http://www.nihstrokescale.org/ .
  27. ^ Litvan, I; Saposnik, G; Mauriño, J; et al. (2000). "Pupillary diameter assessment: need for a graded scale". Neurology. 54 (2): 530–531. doi:10.1212/wnl.54.2.530. PMID 10668738. S2CID 42732541.
  28. ^ a b Olson D, Stutzman S, Saju C, Wilson M, Zhao W, Aiyagari V. Interrater reliability of pupillary assessments. Neurocritical Care. 2015.
  29. ^ a b Stutzman S, Olson D, Saju C, Wilson M, Aiyagari V. Interrater reliability of pupillar assessments among physicians and nurses. UT Southwestern Medical Center; 2014.
  30. ^ Worthley, L (2000). "The pupillary light reflex in the critically ill patient". Critical Care and Resuscitation. 2 (1): 7–8. doi:10.1016/S1441-2772(23)02125-7. PMID 16597274.
  31. ^ Oddo, Mauro; Sandroni, Claudio; Citerio, Giuseppe; Miroz, John-Paul; Horn, Janneke; Rundgren, Malin; Cariou, Alain; Payen, Jean-François; Storm, Christian; Stammet, Pascal; Taccone, Fabio Silvio (December 2018). "Quantitative versus standard pupillary light reflex for early prognostication in comatose cardiac arrest patients: an international prospective multicenter double-blinded study". Intensive Care Medicine. 44 (12): 2102–2111. doi:10.1007/s00134-018-5448-6. ISSN 1432-1238. PMC 6280828. PMID 30478620.
  32. ^ "Pupillometry - NeuroLight | IDMED". 2023-03-06. Retrieved 2023-11-09.
  33. ^ "Vision Development & Rehabilitation". pubs.covd.org. Retrieved 2022-01-31.
  34. ^ Neice, Andrew E.; Fowler, Cedar; Jaffe, Richard A.; Brock-Utne, John G. (December 2021). "Feasibility study of a smartphone pupillometer and evaluation of its accuracy". Journal of Clinical Monitoring and Computing. 35 (6): 1269–1277. doi:10.1007/s10877-020-00592-x. ISSN 1573-2614. PMID 32951188.
  35. ^ McKay, Rachel Eshima; Kohn, Michael A.; Larson, Merlin D. (April 2022). "Pupillary unrest, opioid intensity, and the impact of environmental stimulation on respiratory depression". Journal of Clinical Monitoring and Computing. 36 (2): 473–482. doi:10.1007/s10877-021-00675-3. ISSN 1573-2614. PMC 9123055. PMID 33651243.
  36. ^ Panchal, Ashish R.; Bartos, Jason A.; Cabañas, José G.; Donnino, Michael W.; Drennan, Ian R.; Hirsch, Karen G.; Kudenchuk, Peter J.; Kurz, Michael C.; Lavonas, Eric J.; Morley, Peter T.; O’Neil, Brian J. (2020-10-20). "Part 3: Adult Basic and Advanced Life Support: 2020 American Heart Association Guidelines for Cardiopulmonary Resuscitation and Emergency Cardiovascular Care". Circulation. 142 (16_suppl_2): S366–S468. doi:10.1161/CIR.0000000000000916. ISSN 0009-7322. PMID 33081529.
  37. ^ Aoun, Salah G.; Stutzman, Sonja E.; Vo, Phuong-Uyen N.; El Ahmadieh, Tarek Y.; Osman, Mohamed; Neeley, Om; Plitt, Aaron; Caruso, James P.; Aiyagari, Venkatesh; Atem, Folefac; Welch, Babu G.; White, Jonathan A.; Batjer, H. Hunt; Olson, Daiwai M. (2020). "Detection of delayed cerebral ischemia using objective pupillometry in patients with aneurysmal subarachnoid hemorrhage". Journal of Neurosurgery. 132 (1): 27–32. doi:10.3171/2018.9.JNS181928. PMID 30641848. S2CID 58575267.
  38. ^ Hess, E. H.; Polt, J. M. (1960). "Pupil size as related to interest value of visual stimuli". Science. 132 (3423): 349–350. Bibcode:1960Sci...132..349H. doi:10.1126/science.132.3423.349. PMID 14401489. S2CID 12857616.
  39. ^ Hess, E. H.; Seltzer, A. L.; Shlien, J.M. (1965). "Pupil response of hetero- and homosexual males to pictures of men and women: A pilot study". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 70 (3): 165–168. doi:10.1037/h0021978. PMID 14297654.
  40. ^ Simms, T. M. (1967). "Pupillary response of male and female subjects to pupillary difference in male and female picture stimuli". Perception and Psychophysics. 2 (11): 553–555. doi:10.3758/bf03210265.
  41. ^ a b Goldwater, B. C. (1972). "Psychological significance of pupillary movements" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 77 (5): 340–55. doi:10.1037/h0032456. PMID 5021049.
  42. ^ Nunally, J. C.; Knott, P. D.; Duchnowski, A.; Parker, R. (1967). "Pupillary response as a general measure of activation". Perception and Psychophysics. 2 (4): 149–155. doi:10.3758/BF03210310.
  43. ^ a b Fitzgerald, H. E. (1968). "Autonomic pupillary reflex activity during early infancy and its relation to social and nonsocial visual stimuli". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 6 (3): 470–482. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(68)90127-6. PMID 5687128.
  44. ^ Fitzgerald, H. E.; Lintz, L. M.; Brackbill, Y.; Adams, G. (1967). "Time perception and conditioning an autonomic response in human infants". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 24 (2): 479–486. doi:10.2466/pms.1967.24.2.479. PMID 6068562. S2CID 40269147.
  45. ^ Granholm, E.; Steinhauer, S. R. (2004). "Pupillometric measures of cognitive and emotional processes" (PDF). International Journal of Psychophysiology. 52 (1): 1–6. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.12.001. PMID 15003368.
  46. ^ Vacchiano, R. B.; Strauss, P. S.; Ryan, S.; Hochman, L. (1968). "Pupillary response to value-lined words". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 27 (1): 207–210. doi:10.2466/pms.1968.27.1.207. PMID 5685695. S2CID 38156158.
  47. ^ Simpson, H. M.; Hale, S. M. (1969). "Pupillary Changes During a Decision-Making Task". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 29 (2): 495–498. doi:10.2466/pms.1969.29.2.495. PMID 5361713. S2CID 37552685.
  48. ^ Kahneman, D.; Beatty, J. (1967). "Pupillary Response in a Pitch Discrimination Task". Perception and Psychophysics. 2 (3): 101–105. doi:10.3758/BF03210302.
  49. ^ Kahneman, D.; Beatty, J. (1966). "Pupil Diameter and Load on Memory". Science. 154 (3756): 1583–1585. Bibcode:1966Sci...154.1583K. doi:10.1126/science.154.3756.1583. PMID 5924930. S2CID 22762466.
  50. ^ Hess, E. H.; Polt, J. H. (1964). "Pupil Size in Relation to Mental Activity During Simple Problem Solving". Science. 143 (3611): 1190–1192. Bibcode:1964Sci...143.1190H. doi:10.1126/science.143.3611.1190. PMID 17833905. S2CID 27169110.
  51. ^ Bradshaw, J. L. (1968). "Pupil size and problem solving". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 20 (2): 116–122. doi:10.1080/14640746808400139. PMID 5653414. S2CID 34832644.
  52. ^ Bumke, O. (1911). Die Pupillenstörungen bei den Geistes- und Nervenkrankheiten. Jena: Fischer.
  53. ^ Strauch, C. (2024). "The forgotten wave of early pupillometry research". Trends in Neurosciences. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2024.06.002.
  54. ^ Kafkas, A.; Montaldi, D. (2011). "Recognition memory strength is predicted by pupillary responses at encoding while fixation patterns distinguish recollection from familiarity". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 64 (10): 1971–1989. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.588335. PMID 21838656. S2CID 28231193.
  55. ^ Kafkas, A.; Montaldi, D. (2012). "Familiarity and recollection produce distinct eye movement and pupil and medial temporal lobe responses when memory strength is matched". Neuropsychologia. 50 (13): 3080–93. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.001. PMID 22902538. S2CID 8517388.

Further reading

[edit]